Read The Singularity Is Near: When Humans Transcend Biology Online

Authors: Ray Kurzweil

Tags: #Non-Fiction, #Fringe Science, #Retail, #Technology, #Amazon.com

The Singularity Is Near: When Humans Transcend Biology (31 page)

BOOK: The Singularity Is Near: When Humans Transcend Biology
13.28Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads
 

In one experiment conducted by Michael Merzenich and his colleagues at the University of California at San Francisco, monkeys’ food was placed in such a position that the animals had to dexterously manipulate one finger to obtain it. Brain scans before and after revealed dramatic growth in the interneuronal connections and synapses in the region of the brain responsible for controlling that finger.

Edward Taub at the University of Alabama studied the region of the cortex responsible for evaluating the tactile input from the fingers. Comparing nonmusicians to experienced players of stringed instruments, he found no difference in the brain regions devoted to the fingers of the right hand but a huge difference for the fingers of the left hand. If we drew a picture of the hands based on the amount of brain tissue devoted to analyzing touch, the musicians’ fingers on their left hand (which are used to control the strings) would be huge. Although the difference was greater for those musicians who began musical training with a stringed instrument as children, “even if you take up the violin at 40,” Taub commented, “you still get brain reorganization.”
63

A similar finding comes from an evaluation of a software program, developed by Paula Tallal and Steve Miller at Rutgers University, called Fast ForWord, that assists dyslexic students. The program reads text to children, slowing down staccato phonemes such as “b” and “p,” based on the observation that many dyslexic students are unable to perceive these sounds when spoken quickly. Being read to with this modified form of speech has been shown to help such children learn to read. Using fMRI scanning John Gabrieli of Stanford University found that the left prefrontal region of the brain, an area associated with language processing, had indeed grown and showed greater activity in dyslexic students using the program. Says Tallal, “You create your brain from the input you get.”

It is not even necessary to express one’s thoughts in physical action to provoke the brain to rewire itself. Dr. Alvaro Pascual-Leone at Harvard University scanned the brains of volunteers before and after they practiced a simple piano exercise. The brain motor cortex of the volunteers changed as a direct result of their practice. He then had a second group just think about doing the piano exercise but without actually moving any muscles. This produced an equally pronounced change in the motor-cortex network.
64

Recent fMRI studies of learning visual-spatial relationships found that interneuronal connections are able to change rapidly during the course of a single learning session. Researchers found changes in the connections between posterior parietal-cortex cells in what is called the “dorsal” pathway (which contains information about location and spatial properties of visual stimuli) and posterior inferior-temporal cortex cells in the “ventral” pathway (which contains recognized invariant features of varying levels of abstraction);
65
significantly, that rate of change was directly proportional to the rate of learning.
66

Researchers at the University of California at San Diego reported a key insight into the difference in the formation of short-term and long-term memories. Using a high-resolution scanning method, the scientists were able to see chemical changes within synapses in the hippocampus, the brain region associated with the formation of long-term memories.
67
They discovered that when a cell was first stimulated, actin, a neurochemical, moved toward the neurons to which the synapse was connected. This also stimulated the actin in neighboring cells to move away from the activated cell. These changes lasted only a few minutes, however. If the stimulations were sufficiently repeated, then a more significant and permanent change took place.

“The short-term changes are just part of the normal way the nerve cells talk to each other,” lead author Michael A. Colicos said.

The long-term changes in the neurons occur only after the neurons are stimulated four times over the course of an hour. The synapse will
actually split and new synapses will form, producing a permanent change that will presumably last for the rest of your life. The analogy to human memory is that when you see or hear something once, it might stick in your mind for a few minutes. If it’s not important, it fades away and you forget it 10 minutes later. But if you see or hear it again and this keeps happening over the next hour, you are going to remember it for a much longer time. And things that are repeated many times can be remembered for an entire lifetime. Once you take an axon and form two new connections, those connections are very stable and there’s no reason to believe that they’ll go away. That’s the kind of change one would envision lasting a whole lifetime.

“It’s like a piano lesson,” says coauthor and professor of biology Yukiko Goda. “If you play a musical score over and over again, it becomes ingrained in your memory.” Similarly, in an article in
Science
neuroscientists S. Lowel and W. Singer report having found evidence for rapid dynamic formation of new interneuronal connections in the visual cortex, which they described with Donald Hebb’s phrase “What fires together wires together.”
68

Another insight into memory formation is reported in a study published in
Cell
. Researchers found that the CPEB protein actually changes its shape in synapses to record memories.
69
The surprise was that CPEB performs this memory function while in a prion state.

“For a while we’ve known quite a bit about how memory works, but we’ve had no clear concept of what the key storage device is,” said coauthor and Whitehead Institute for Biomedical Research director Susan Lindquist. “This study suggests what the storage device might be—but it’s such a surprising suggestion to find that a prion like activity may be involved. . . . It . . . indicates that prions aren’t just oddballs of nature but might participate in fundamental processes.” As I reported in
chapter 3
, human engineers are also finding prions to be a powerful means of building electronic memories.

Brain-scanning studies are also revealing mechanisms to inhibit unneeded and undesirable memories, a finding that would gratify Sigmund Freud.
70
Using fMRI, Stanford University scientists asked study subjects to attempt to forget information that they had earlier memorized. During this activity, regions in the frontal cortex that have been associated with memory repression showed a high level of activity, while the hippocampus, the region normally associated with remembering, was relatively inactive. These findings “confirm the existence of an active forgetting process and establish a neurobiological model for guiding inquiry into motivated forgetting,” wrote Stanford psychology professor John Gabrieli and his colleagues. Gabrieli also commented,
“The big news is that we’ve shown how the human brain blocks an unwanted memory, that there is such a mechanism, and it has a biological basis. It gets you past the possibility that there’s nothing in the brain that would suppress a memory—that it was all a misunderstood fiction.”

In addition to generating new connections between neurons, the brain also makes new neurons from neural stem cells, which replicate to maintain a reservoir of themselves. In the course of reproducing, some of the neural stem cells become “neural precursor” cells, which in turn mature into two types of support cells called astrocytes and oligodendrocytes, as well as neurons. The cells further evolve into specific types of neurons. However, this differentiation cannot take place unless the neural stem cells move away from their original source in the brain’s ventricles. Only about half of the neural cells successfully make the journey, which is similar to the process during gestation and early childhood in which only a portion of the early brain’s developing neurons survive. Scientists hope to bypass this neural migration process by injecting neural stem cells directly into target regions, as well as to create drugs that promote this process of neurogenesis (creating new neurons) to repair brain damage from injury or disease.
71

An experiment by genetics researchers Fred Gage, G. Kempermann, and Henriette van Praag at the Salk Institute for Biological Studies showed that neurogenesis is actually stimulated by our experience. Moving mice from a sterile, uninteresting cage to a stimulating one approximately doubled the number of dividing cells in their hippocampus regions.
72

Modeling Regions of the Brain

 

Most probably the human brain is, in the main, composed of large numbers of relatively small distributed systems, arranged by embryology into a complex society that is controlled in part (but only in part) by serial, symbolic systems that are added later. But the subsymbolic systems that do most of the work from underneath must, by their very character, block all the other parts of the brain from knowing much about how they work. And this, itself, could help explain how people do so many things yet have such incomplete ideas on how those things are actually done.

                   —M
ARVIN
M
INSKY AND
S
EYMOUR
P
APERT
73

 

Common sense is not a simple thing. Instead, it is an immense society of hard-earned practical ideas—of multitudes of life-learned rules and exceptions, dispositions and tendencies, balances and checks.

                   —M
ARVIN
M
INSKY

 

In addition to new insights into the plasticity of organization of each brain region, researchers are rapidly creating detailed models of particular regions of the brain. These neuromorphic models and simulations lag only slightly behind the availability of the information on which they are based. The rapid success of turning the detailed data from studies of neurons and the interconnection data from neural scanning into effective models and working simulations belies often-stated skepticism about our inherent capability of understanding our own brains.

Modeling human-brain functionality on a nonlinearity-by-nonlinearity and synapse-by-synapse basis is generally not necessary. Simulations of regions that store memories and skills in individual neurons and connections (for example, the cerebellum) do make use of detailed cellular models. Even for these regions, however, simulations require far less computation than is implied by all of the neural components. This is true of the cerebellum simulation described below.

Although there is a great deal of detailed complexity and nonlinearity in the subneural parts of each neuron, as well as a chaotic, semirandom wiring pattern underlying the trillions of connections in the brain, significant progress has been made over the past twenty years in the mathematics of modeling such adaptive nonlinear systems. Preserving the exact shape of every dendrite and the precise “squiggle” of every interneuronal connection is generally not necessary. We can understand the principles of operation of extensive regions of the brain by examining their dynamics at the appropriate level of analysis.

We have already had significant success in creating models and simulations of extensive brain regions. Applying tests to these simulations and comparing the data to that obtained from psychophysical experiments on actual human brains have produced impressive results. Given the relative crudeness of our scanning and sensing tools to date, the success in modeling, as illustrated by the following works in progress, demonstrates the ability to extract the right insights from the mass of data being gathered.

The following are only a few examples of successful models of brain regions, all works in progress.

A Neuromorphic Model: The Cerebellum

 

A question I examined in
The Age of Spiritual Machines
is: how does a ten-year-old manage to catch a fly ball?
74
All that a child can see is the ball’s trajectory from his position in the outfield. To actually infer the path of the ball in three-dimensional space would require solving difficult simultaneous differential
equations. Additional equations would need to be solved to predict the future course of the ball, and more equations to translate these results into what was required of the player’s own movements. How does a young outfielder accomplish all of this in a few seconds with no computer and no training in differential equations? Clearly, he is not solving equations consciously, but how does his brain solve the problem?

Since
ASM
was published, we have advanced considerably in understanding this basic process of skill formation. As I had hypothesized, the problem is not solved by building a mental model of three-dimensional motion. Rather, the problem is collapsed by directly translating the observed movements of the ball into the appropriate movement of the player and changes in the configuration of his arms and legs. Alexandre Pouget of the University of Rochester and Lawrence H. Snyder of Washington University have described mathematical “basis functions” that can represent this direct transformation of perceived movement in the visual field to required movements of the muscles.
75
Furthermore, analysis of recently developed models of the functioning of the cerebellum demonstrate that our cerebellar neural circuits are indeed capable of learning and then applying the requisite basis functions to implement these sensorimotor transformations. When we engage in the trial-and-error process of learning to perform a sensorimotor task, such as catching a fly ball, we are training the synaptic potentials of the cerebellar synapses to learn the appropriate basis functions. The cerebellum performs two types of transformations with these basis functions: going from a desired result to an action (called “inverse internal models”) and going from a possible set of actions to an anticipated result (“forward internal models”). Tomaso Poggio has pointed out that the idea of basis functions may describe learning processes in the brain that go beyond motor control.
76

BOOK: The Singularity Is Near: When Humans Transcend Biology
13.28Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Other books

Princess for Hire by Lindsey Leavitt
Harbinger by Cyndi Friberg
Redzone by William C. Dietz
Return to Oakpine by Ron Carlson
Another Woman's House by Mignon G. Eberhart
Wifey 4 Life by Kiki Swinson
Spent (Wrecked #2) by Charity Parkerson
House Guest by Ron Dawes