Authors: Daniel Bor
N. Pratt, A. Willoughby, and D. Swick, Effects of working memory load on visual selective attention: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence.
Front Hum Neurosci
, 2011. 5: 57.
E. I. Knudsen, Fundamental components of attention.
Annu Rev Neurosci
, 2007. 30: 57–78.C. Bundesen, T. Habekost, and S. Kyllingsbaek, A neural theory of visual attention: bridging cognition and neurophysiology.
Psychol Rev
, 2005. 112(2): 291–328.
D. Bor et al., Encoding strategies dissociate prefrontal activity from working memory demand.
Neuron
, 2003. 37(2): 361–367.
D. Bor et al., Prefrontal cortical involvement in verbal encoding strategies.
Eur J Neurosci
, 2004. 19(12): 3365–3370.
D. Bor and A. M. Owen, A common prefrontal-parietal network for mnemonic and mathematical recoding strategies within working memory.
Cereb Cortex
, 2007. 17(4): 778–786.
C. R. Savage et al., Prefrontal regions supporting spontaneous and directed application of verbal learning strategies: evidence from PET.
Brain
, 2001. 124 (Pt 1): 219–231.
V. Prabhakaran et al., Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe.
Nat Neurosci
, 2000. 3(1): 85–90.
C. D. Moore, M. X. Cohen, and C. Ranganath, Neural mechanisms of expert skills in visual working memory.
J. Neurosci
, 2006. 26(43): 11187–11196.
C. Landmann et al., Dynamics of prefrontal and cingulate activity during a reward-based logical deduction task.
Cereb Cortex
, 2007. 17(4): 749–759.
S. Baron-Cohen et al., Savant memory in a man with colour form-number synaesthesia and Asperger syndrome.
J Consciousness Studies
, 2007. 14(9–10): 237–251.
Ibid.
D. Bor, J. Billington, and S. Baron-Cohen, Savant memory for digits in a case of synaesthesia and Asperger syndrome is related to hyperactivity in the lateral prefrontal cortex.
Neurocase
, 2007. 13(5): 311–319.
D. Bor and A. K. Seth, Consciousness and the prefrontal parietal network: insights from attention, working memory and chunking.
Front Psychol,
2012. 3: 63.
F. Crick,
The astonishing hypothesis: the search for the scientific soul
. 1994, New York: Scribner.
H. Tiitinen et al., Selective attention enhances the auditory 40-Hz transient response in humans.
Nature
, 1993. 364(6432): 59–60.
C. H. Vanderwolf, Are neocortical gamma waves related to consciousness?
Brain Res
, 2000. 855(2): 217–224.
R. T. Canolty et al., High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex.
Science
, 2006. 313(5793): 1626–1628.R. Gaillard et al., Converging intracranial markers of conscious access. PLoS Biol, 2009. 7(3): e61.
Bundesen et al. (2005), see above.Gaillard et al. (2009), see above.
A. Maier, C. J. Aura, and D. A. Leopold, Infragranular sources of sustained local field potential responses in macaque primary visual cortex.
J Neurosci
, 2011. 31(6): 1971–1980.
V.A.F. Lamme, How neuroscience will change our view on consciousness.
Cogn Neurosci
, 2010. 1(3): 204–220.
Dehaene and Changeux (2011), see above.S. Dehaene, M. Kerszberg, and J. P. Changeux, A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
, 1998. 95(24): 14529–14534.
D. S. Modha and R. Singh, Network architecture of the long-distance pathways in the macaque brain.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 2010.
107(30): 13485–13490.
G. Tononi, An information integration theory of consciousness.
BMC Neurosci
, 2004. 5: 42.G. Tononi, Consciousness as integrated information: a provisional manifesto.
Biol Bull
, 2008. 215(3): 216–242.
A. K. Seth et al., Theories and measures of consciousness: an extended framework.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
, 2006. 103(28): 10799–10804.
Bor and Seth (2012), see above.
S. Savage-Rumbaugh and R. Lewin,
Kanzi: the ape at the brink of the human mind
. 1994, New York: John Wiley & Sons.
D. McFarland,
Guilty robots, happy dogs: the question of alien minds
. 2008, Oxford: Oxford University Press.
F. Wemelsfelder, Animal boredom: understanding the tedium of confined lives, in
Mental health and well-being in animals
, F. D. McMillan, ed. 2005, Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
N. J. Emery and N. S. Clayton, The mentality of crows: convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes.
Science
, 2004. 306(5703): 1903–1907.
C. R. Raby et al., Planning for the future by western scrub-jays.
Nature
, 2007. 445(7130): 919–921.
J. M. Dally, N. J. Emery, and N. S. Clayton, Food-caching western scrub-jays keep track of who was watching when.
Science
, 2006. 312(5780): 1662–1665.
J. H. Wimpenny et al., Cognitive processes associated with sequential tool use in New Caledonian crows. PLoS One, 2009. 4(8): e6471.
C. D. Bird and N. J. Emery, Rooks use stones to raise the water level to reach a floating worm.
Current Biol
, 2009. 19(16): 1410–1414.
D. Hanus et al., Comparing the performances of apes (
Gorilla gorilla, Pan troglodytes, Pongo pygmaeus
) and human children (
Homo sapiens
) in the floating peanut task. PLoS One, 2011. 6(6): e19555.
N. Mendes, D. Hanus, and J. Call, Raising the level: orangutans use water as a tool.
Biol Lett
, 2007. 3(5): 453–455.
D. B. Edelman and A. K. Seth, Animal consciousness: a synthetic approach.
Trends Neurosci
, 2009. 32(9): 476–484.
N. K. Logothetis, Single units and conscious vision.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
, 1998. 353(1377): 1801–1818.
N. Kornell, L. K. Son, and H. S. Terrace, Transfer of metacognitive skills and hint seeking in monkeys.
Psychol Sci
, 2007. 18(1): 64–71.
R. Kiani and M. N. Shadlen, Representation of confidence associated with a decision by neurons in the parietal cortex.
Science
, 2009. 324(5928): 759–764.
C. Suda-King, Do orangutans (
Pongo pygmaeus
) know when they do not remember?
Anim Cogn
, 2008. 11(1): 21–42.A. L. Foote and J. D. Crystal, Metacognition in the rat.
Curr Biol
, 2007. 17(6): 551–555.
T. Macuda and W. A. Roberts, Further evidence for hierarchical chunking in rat spatial memory.
J Exp Psych: Anim Behav Proc
, 1995. 21(1): 20–32.
H. S. Terrace, Chunking by a pigeon in a serial learning task.
Nature
, 1987. 325(7000): 149–151.