The Ravenous Brain: How the New Science of Consciousness Explains Our Insatiable Search for Meaning (50 page)

BOOK: The Ravenous Brain: How the New Science of Consciousness Explains Our Insatiable Search for Meaning
13.53Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
D. J. Simons and D. T. Levin, Failure to detect changes to people during a real-world interaction.
Psychon Bull Rev
, 1998. 5(4): 644–649.
 
116
44 percent of people actually notice the gorilla
D. J. Simons and C. F. Chabris, Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events.
Perception
, 1999. 28(9): 1059–1074.
 
117
Conscious of fainter targets . . . also detect targets faster
R. Desimone and J. Duncan, Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention.
Annu Rev Neurosci
, 1995. 18: 193–222.
J. W. Bisley and M. E. Goldberg, Neuronal activity in the lateral intraparietal area and spatial attention.
Science
, 2003. 299(5603): 81–86.
M. Carrasco, C. Penpeci-Talgar, and M. Eckstein, Spatial covert attention increases contrast sensitivity across the CSF: support for signal enhancement.
Vision Res
, 2000. 40(10–12): 1203–1215.
 
117
Object will be perceived . . . more contrast
M. Carrasco, S. Ling, and S. Read, Attention alters appearance.
Nat Neurosci
, 2004. 7(3): 308–313.
 
118
Attention emerging from . . . collective neuronal war
Desimone and Duncan, Neural mechanisms.
J. Duncan, EPS Mid-Career Award 2004: brain mechanisms of attention.
Q J Exp Psychol
(Colchester), 2006. 59(1): 2–27.
 
119
Famous student physics lectures
R. P. Feynman, R. B. Leighton, and M. Sands,
Lectures on physics: complete set
, vols. 1–3. 1998, Boston: Addison Wesley.
 
122
Ferret brains . . . rewired . . . visual cortex in blind . . . process Braille
J. Sharma, A. Angelucci, and M. Sur, Induction of visual orientation modules in auditory cortex.
Nature
, 2000. 404(6780): 841–847.
L. Von Melchner, S. L. Pallas, and M. Sur, Visual behaviour mediated by retinal projections directed to the auditory pathway.
Nature
, 2000. 404(6780): 871–876.
H. Burton et al., Adaptive changes in early and late blind: a fMRI study of Braille reading.
J Neurophysiol
, 2002. 87(1): 589–607.
 
124
Constant competition . . . in the brain
Desimone and Duncan (1995), see above.
Duncan (2006), see above.
E. I. Knudsen, Fundamental components of attention.
Annu Rev Neurosci
, 2007. 30: 57–78.
 
127
Modern version of Phineas Gage
A. R. Damasio,
Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain
. 1994, New York: HarperCollins.
 
129
Multiple personality disorder . . . attribute . . . experiences to other personalities
E. Bliss,
Multiple personality, allied disorders, and hypnosis
. 1986, Oxford: Oxford University Press.
 
131
Aware of your own consciousness
H. Lau and D. Rosenthal, Empirical support for higher-order theories of conscious awareness.
Trends Cogn Sci
, 2011. 15(8): 365–373.
 
132
Poor at matching their confidence to their accuracy
S. M. Fleming et al., Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in brain structure.
Science
, 2010. 329(5998): 1541–1543.
 
135
Three or four conscious items
N. Cowan, The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity.
Behav Brain Sci
, 2001. 24(1): 87–114; discussion 114–185.
 
135
“Global workspace theory” proposed by Bernard Baars
B. J. Baars,
In the theater of consciousness: the workspace of the mind
. 1997, New York: Oxford University Press.
B. J. Baars and S. Franklin, An architectural model of conscious and unconscious brain functions: Global Workspace Theory and IDA.
Neural Netw
, 2007. 20(9): 955–961.
 
136
George Sperling presented subjects
G. Sperling, The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Monographs, 1960. 74(11, Whole No. 498): 1–29.
 
137
Steven Yantis presented subjects with
S. Yantis, Multielement visual tracking: attention and perceptual organization.
Cogn Psychol
, 1992. 24(3): 295–340.
 
137
Our working memory limit . . . same as the monkey’s
E. Heyselaar, K. Johnston, and M. Paré, The capacity limit of the visual working memory of the macaque monkey.
J Vision
. 10(7): 725.
 
138
Other species . . . same upper bound . . . honeybee
H. J. Gross et al., Number-based visual generalisation in the honeybee. PLoS One, 2009. 4(1): e4263.
 
138
3 or 4 items . . . maximum that can be practically sustained
A. Raffone and G. Wolters, A cortical mechanism for binding in visual working memory.
J Cogn Neurosci
, 2001. 13(6): 766–785.
 
139
Each holder . . . cope equally well . . . simplest . . . most complex
J. Duncan, Similarity between concurrent visual discriminations: dimensions and objects.
Percept Psychophys
, 1993. 54(4): 425–430.
 
140
Say back a novel sequence that was 80 digits
K. A. Ericcson, W. G. Chase, and S. Falloon, Acquisition of a memory skill.
Science
, 1980. 208: 1181–1182.
 
142
Presented volunteers . . . sequences of 4 double digits
D. Bor and A. M. Owen, A common prefrontal-parietal network for mnemonic and mathematical recoding strategies within working memory.
Cereb Cortex
, 2007. 17(4): 778–786.
 
142
Chess masters . . . remember . . . whole board
W. G. Chase and H. A. Simon, Perception in chess.
Cogn Psychol
, 1973. 4: 55–81.
 
143
Increase the amount of information per item
G. A. Miller, The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information.
Psychol Rev
, 1956. 63(2): 81–97.
 
144
Greater the number, the less likely . . . identify each
C. Bundesen, H. Shibuya, and A. Larsen, Visual selection from multielement displays: a model for partial report, in
Attention and performance XI
, M. I. Posner and M.S.I. Marin, eds. 1985, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 631–649.
J. J. Todd and R. Marois, Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex.
Nature
, 2004. 428(6984): 751–754.
 
144
Attention has two clear stages
C. Bundesen, T. Habekost, and S. Kyllingsbaek, A neural theory of visual attention: bridging cognition and neurophysiology.
Psychol Rev
, 2005. 112(2): 291–328.
 
145
Leonardo Chelazzi and colleagues used electrodes
L. Chelazzi et al., Responses of neurons in inferior temporal cortex during memory-guided visual search.
J Neurophysiol
, 1998. 80(6): 2918–2940.
 
145
Michael Cohen and colleagues recently carried out
M. A. Cohen, G. A. Alvarez, and K. Nakayama, Natural-scene perception requires attention.
Psychol Sci
, 2011. 22(9): 1165–1172.
 
146
Divert attention away . . . fails to enter consciousness
J. S. Joseph, M. M. Chun, and K. Nakayama, Attentional requirements in a “preattentive” feature search task.
Nature
, 1997. 387(6635): 805–807.
J. Theeuwes, A. F. Kramer, and P. Atchley, Attentional effects on preattentive vision: spatial precues affect the detection of simple features.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform
, 1999. 25(2): 341–347.
S. Walker, P. Stafford, and G. Davis, Ultra-rapid categorization requires visual attention: scenes with multiple foreground objects.
J Vision
, 2008. 8(4): 211–212.
 
146
Working memory . . . is limited to . . . 4 . . . items
Cowan (2001), see above.
 
147
Psychologists assumed that our working memory capacity was around double this
Miller (1956), see above.
 
148
Douglas Hofstadter’s whimsical and influential book
D. Hofstadter,
Godel, Escher, Bach: an eternal golden braid (20th anniversary edition, with a new preface by the author)
. 1999, London: Penguin.
 
150
Prior expectations . . . guide our attention
L. Melloni et al., Expectations change the signatures and timing of electrophysiological correlates of perceptual awareness.
J Neurosci
, 2011. 31(4): 1386–1396.
C. Summerfield and T. Egner, Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition.
Trends Cogn Sci
, 2009. 13(9): 403–409.
 
151
Other species can start to learn . . . grammatical language
S. Savage-Rumbaugh and R. Lewin,
Kanzi: the ape at the brink of the human mind
. 1994, New York: John Wiley & Sons.
 
151
A “language instinct”
S. Pinker,
The language instinct: the new science of language and mind
. 2003, London: Penguin.
 
152
Learning an artificial grammar . . . same brain areas . . . chunking task
D. Bor et al., Encoding strategies dissociate prefrontal activity from working memory demand.
Neuron
, 2003. 37(2): 361–367.
P. Fletcher et al., Learning-related neuronal responses in prefrontal cortex studied with functional neuroimaging.
Cereb Cortex
, 1999. 9(2): 168–178.
 
152
FOXP2
F. Vargha-Khadem et al., FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language
. Nat Rev Neurosci
, 2005. 6(2): 131–138.
 
154
The evolutionary advantage of awareness
D. Bor and A. K. Seth, Consciousness and the prefrontal parietal network: insights from attention, working memory and chunking.
Front Psychol,
2012. 3: 63.
 
155
Stan Beilock and colleagues tested . . . goft-putting
S. L. Beilock et al., Haste does not always make waste: expertise, direction of attention, and speed versus accuracy in performing sensorimotor skills.
Psychon Bull Rev
, 2004. 11(2): 373–379.
S. L. Beilock et al., When paying attention becomes counterproductive: impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills.
J Exp Psychol Appl
, 2002. 8(1): 6–16.

Other books

The Suicide Club by Gayle Wilson
This Fierce Splendor by Iris Johansen
Shadow Horse by Alison Hart
Love Me and Die by Louis Trimble
Davy Crockett by Robert E. Hollmann
Hellhound on My Trail by D. J. Butler
Summer in Tuscany by Elizabeth Adler