Manufacturing depression (46 page)

Read Manufacturing depression Online

Authors: Gary Greenberg

BOOK: Manufacturing depression
3.24Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

Still, however, as Amen goes on in his calm and confident way about empty cerebellums and hypoactive cingulates, too much serotonin and not enough dopamine, trauma diamonds and depression triads, and the bounty that awaits me when he balances my brain—“With a healthy brain,” he tells me, “your free will is greater, and so is your ability to have people trust you over time, be able to be engaged in a loving long-term relationship, be a good dad, to be more thoughtful and loving”—it’s impossible to shake the feeling that Daniel Amen is a high-tech, nuclear-armed quack and that I’ve stumbled into a twenty-first-century medicine show.

I’ll admit that this sentiment is not only uncharitable—not that he needs my charity, not at $3,250 a pop, not to mention the best-sellers and the lecture tours and the television specials—but it’s awfully convenient, self-serving even. I mean, it’s not like I didn’t go into this meeting with a deep suspicion of the project of rendering human life as a series of biochemical events, and of Thomas Insel’s vision of the psychiatry of the future. For that matter, it’s not like I didn’t go into this book without an idea about what is wrong with the medical industry’s invention of a brain disease out of our daily troubles and aspirations. After all, just because he’s a little too slick and greedy for my tastes, that doesn’t mean he’s wrong.

Amen has met the likes of me before—people who don’t want to accept the morphing of psychiatry into clinical neuroscience, of the mind into the brain, of discontents into glitches. “When I first started doing this work,” he told me, “I got no end of grief from my colleagues.” So, he says, he learned to shut up, speaking very little about what he was up to and only to what he thought would be sympathetic audiences. But in 1996, he gave a talk to a professional group, and one audience member, incensed, Amen says, by
his claims to have discerned the physical foundations of mental illness, hauled him before the California Medical Board. He complained that Amen was practicing outside the standards of care, using equipment that only neurologists had the training to use—an offense for which a doctor can lose his license. Amen battled the board for more than a year and finally was cleared of the charges—but not before he realized what he was up against. “
One is often
labeled a heretic for trying to change religious beliefs,” he says.

 

But compared to what the French and Dutch authorities did to Julien Offray de La Mettrie in the eighteenth century for suggesting that consciousness was the product of a machine-like brain, litigation with a medical board is a walk in the park. Similarly, my showing up here with a check, a brain, and my skepticism doesn’t seem so bad compared to what Mark Twain did to the American phrenologist Lorenzo Fowler in 1873.

By then, America had proven itself a hospitable home to the brand of phrenology originated by Franz Gall’s assistant
Johann Spurzheim, who had redrawn
the skull map in a way that eliminated the bad bits. Spurzheim had also declared that evil and suffering were not inherent properties of the brain but the result of pathologies that could be corrected. He did well enough in Europe, but Spurzheim’s science of self-improvement set off an outright frenzy in the land dedicated to the pursuit of happiness. Spurzheim arrived in New York in August 1832 and lectured in New Haven and Hartford before arriving in Boston to a reception so enthusiastic that he soon found himself lecturing to overflow crowds at the Masonic Temple, to Harvard students and faculty, and at the Boston Medical Society. It was a killer pace—literally. He died at the end of September of exhaustion and fever.

Those two months, however, were long enough to capture the attention of a young Amherst student and aspiring minister named
Orson Squire Fowler
. By the time he had graduated in 1834, Fowler had examined heads in and around Amherst with such success that he abandoned his plans for the ministry and became a full time phrenologist.
With his brother Lorenzo, he married Gall’s brain maps and Spurzheim’s optimism to American pragmatism and took the resulting self-help science to the streets, offering readings intended to help common people learn “
what they
are
, and what they
can
be as well as how to make themselves what they should become.” What Gall had eschewed, what Spurzheim had only implied, the Fowlers said right out in their phrenological self-instructor, one of America’s first self-help books: the purpose of life is happiness, which is achieved when the organism—specifically the brain—is functioning as it should. “
Would you become great mentally
,” they wrote, “then first become great cerebrally.” Or as Daniel Amen would say a century and a half later, change your brain and change your life.

The Fowlers built
a therapeutic empire
that eventually included not only the brothers but their sister, her husband, Lorenzo’s wife (who in 1850 became the second American woman to earn a medical degree), and their daughter. They set up shop in New York City, with satellite offices in Philadelphia, Boston, and even London.
President James Garfield
, Brigham Young, John Brown, and Oliver Wendell Holmes all offered their skulls to the Fowlers’ hands. Like everyone else, these luminaries went home with charts of their heads, on which their strengths were outlined in black and white for all the world to see.

By midcentury, phrenology had reached into most areas of American public life. But it was a particular hit with the literati.
Horace Greeley published
Combe’s lectures and suggested that railroad men be selected on the basis of the shape of their heads. Horace Mann called phrenology “
the greatest discovery
of the age.” Edgar Allan Poe wrote that “
Phrenology… has assumed
the majesty of a science, and, as a science ranks among the most important which can engage the attention of thinking beings,” and built a poetics on phrenological tenets. Ralph Waldo Emerson spoke favorably, if grudgingly, about the Fowlers’ achievements.

But no literary figure was more effusive than Walt Whitman. “
Phrenology, it must be confessed
by all men who have open eyes,”
he wrote, “has at last gained a position, and a firm one, among the sciences.”
The relationship between Whitman and the Fowlers
was a mutual lovefest. He sang their multitudinous praises—inspired, no doubt, by Lorenzo’s reading of his skull, which proved him to be well endowed in the areas of Friendship, Sympathy, and Self-esteem, not to mention Voluptuousness and Amativeness—and Fowler & Wells, phrenology’s publishing arm, published his poems, including
Leaves of Grass
, which they then allowed him to review (anonymously, of course) in the
American Phrenological Journal.
Whitman gave the book a favorable notice.

We can only imagine how all this piety and respect sounded to a skeptic like Mark Twain, although his telling makes it clear that his motives were no purer than Joseph Wortis’s or David Rosenhan’s (or, of course, mine):

I made a small test
of phrenology for my better information. I went to [Lorenzo] Fowler under an assumed name. He examined my elevations and depressions, and he gave me a chart… He said I possessed amazing courage, an abnormal spirit of daring, a pluck, stern will, a fearlessness that were without limit. I was simply astonished at this, and gratified too; I had not suspected it before… However, he found a
CAVITY
in one place where a bump should have been in anybody else’s skull… He startled me by saying that that
CAVITY
represented a total absence of a “Sense of Humor”!

 

Twain went back to Fowler three months later, this time “bearing both my name and my
nom de guerre
.” This time the news was different:

 

The
CAVITY
was gone, and in its place was a Mount Everest—figuratively speaking—31,000 feet high, the loftiest
BUMP OF HUMOR
he had ever encountered in his life-long experience!

 

It wasn’t Twain’s prank that undid phrenology, however, but rather the advent of scientific medicine, especially the nascent field of neurology. In 1848, for instance, a railroad worker in Vermont, Phineas Gage, was loading a hole with gunpowder when an errant spark set off the charge, sending the tamping iron through Gage’s skull, carrying with it a good portion of Gage’s frontal cortex. Gage was brought to a local physician, John Harlow, who treated him with the usual purgatives and poisons, amputated the herniated brain tissue, and closed his skull as best he could. Gage floated in and out of consciousness for a couple of weeks, but he was soon up and around and ready to get his job back. But the railroad didn’t want him. As Harlow reported, Gage was a changed man. “
The equilibrium or balance
… between his intellectual faculties and animal propensities,” he wrote in a medical journal, “seems to have been destroyed.” Gage, who was once a gentle and reliable man, had become irascible, unpredictable, and even mean—so much so that his friends, according to Harlow, reported that “Gage was no longer Gage.” Gage left Vermont, worked as a coach driver in Chile, and died in San Francisco in 1860.

Harlow was sympathetic to phrenology, but
he didn’t report the fact
that Gage was a changed man until 1868—perhaps because the areas of the brain wiped out by the tamping iron, according to Gall’s charts, governed functions like poetical talent and acquisitiveness, and not equanimity or amativeness. But by then, other discoveries had called the phrenological brain map into question. In 1861, for instance, Paul Broca, a doctor in Paris, autopsied an epileptic man who, before he died of gangrene, had lost the power of speech. He found a “softening” in an area at the rear of the left frontal lobe, a lesion that showed up subsequently in other people who could not speak.
Broca’s area
, as that section of the brain came to be known, was soon joined in the brain gazetteer by
Wernicke’s area
—a region discovered by German neurologist Carl Wernicke that, when damaged, causes receptive aphasia, the loss of the ability to understand speech. And in the 1870s
David Ferrier
, a British doctor experimenting
on monkeys, found that frontal cortex damage like Gage suffered could induce profound changes in behavior.

These discoveries didn’t kill phrenology, not entirely. Doctors digging around in brains may have discredited the idea that the skull revealed the brain beneath it, but they also confirmed Gall’s basic insight (or maybe it was just a lucky guess): that the brain is divided into regions, and that those regions correspond to various functions. Gall was wrong in the particulars, but right that the brain, much as La Mettrie had said, was an elaborate clockworks.

At least that’s the story
that historians of medicine like to tell: that real scientists managed to distill the gold of neurology from the dross of phrenology, Broca’s epistemological modesty triumphing over the Fowlers’ hubris, real doctors vanquishing the quacks. But the Fowlers’ version lives on, and not only at Daniel Amen’s clinic, where anyone can plunk down some cash and come away with a picture of the bumps on the inside of his head, a reading of their significance, and a program for brain improvement. It’s in the inescapable images in every magazine and newspaper and television program about health, the full-color scans showing the brain at work and relaying the news that doctors have planted the flag of science in our brains like sixteenth-century explorers, that they have discovered the real basis of
empathy
or
racial prejudice
or
sexual orientation
, claiming for the brain territories once thought to be the province of the mind.

Especially depression. Pick up
the October 16, 2008 issue of
Nature
—okay, it’s not exactly light reading, but it is about as mainstream science as it gets—and you’ll find out just how far into the brain the depression doctors have penetrated. After you hear from two of the leading lights in the field that “the official diagnosis of depression is subjective” and that its cause is “far from being a simple deficiency of monoamines”—once again, doctors can acknowledge to one another that what they tell their patients is not true—you’ll learn
that depression can be found in the
grey-matter volume
and
glial density
of the
prefrontal cortex
and the
hippocampus
, in the
amygdala
and
subgenual cingulate cortex
and the
nucleus accumbens
. You’ll find that the reason SSRIs work, when they work, may be that they cause
secondary neuroplastic changes
, themselves perhaps the result of
upregulation
of the
calcium-binding protein p11
or the
transcription factor CREB
, which, following the logic of psychopharmacology, indicates that those are the causes of depression. Or maybe the culprit is a lack of
brain-derived neurotrophic factor
or
vascular endothelial growth factor
leading to a decrease in
hippocampal neurogenesis
, or too many
cytokines
or
glucocorticoids
floating around the brain or just glitches in the
intracellular interactions between brain macrophages
. You’ll also discover that
resilience
, the ability to withstand stress and adversity that the depression doctors define as health, may be due to a good supply of
ΔFOSB
, especially in the
midbrain periaqueductal grey nucleus
and a relative lack of
Substance P
, or even a reduction in the stress-related
increased excitability of VTA dopamine neurons
caused by
upregulating voltage-gated potassium channels
.

 

And if that doesn’t convince you that doctors are hot on depression’s trail, take a look at the
New England Journal of Medicine
, in which
a team of French doctors reported
that a very strange thing happened when they were treating a sixty-five-year-old woman with deep brain stimulation (DBS) for her Parkinson’s disease. That procedure involves inserting tiny electrodes into carefully chosen individual neurons and turning on a low-voltage electrical current, which generally relieves the tremors and spasticity associated with Parkinson’s. In this case, within a few seconds of receiving the charge, the woman, who had previously shown no signs of depression, started to cry. “I no longer wish to live, to see anything, to feel anything,” she said. “I’m fed up with life, I’ve had enough.” And in case her doctors missed the point, she continued, “Everything is useless, always feeling worthless, I’m hopeless, why am I bothering you?” Ninety seconds after the stimulator was shut off, her depression disappeared.

Other books

The Jezebel's Daughter by Juliet MacLeod
Where You Are by J.H. Trumble
Arkwright by Allen Steele
Outside Chance by Lyndon Stacey
Red Cell by Mark Henshaw