How Many Friends Does One Person Need?

BOOK: How Many Friends Does One Person Need?
7.89Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

How many friends does one person need?

Robin Dunbar

How Many Friends Does One Person Need?

Dunbar’s Number and Other Evolutionary Quirks

Harvard University Press

Cambridge, Massachusetts

2010

Copyright © 2010 by Robin Dunbar

All rights reserved

Printed in the United States of America

First published in 2010 in the United Kingdom

by Faber and Faber Limited

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data

Dunbar, R. I. M. (Robin Ian MacDonald), 1947–

How many friends does one person need? : Dunbar’s number and other evolutionary quirks / Robin Dunbar.

p. cm.

Includes index.

ISBN 978-0-674-05716-6 (cloth : alk. paper)

1. Social psychology. 2. Human behavior. 3. Evolution.

I. Title.

HM1033.D857 2010

599.93′8—dc22 2010029306

Contents

 

 

Acknowledgements

This volume had its origins in a series of popular science articles that I wrote for
New Scientist
magazine (mostly between 1994 and 2006) and the
Scotsman
newspaper (between 2005 and 2008). In bringing them together in this volume, my intention has been to give some flavour of the excitement – and some of the fun – that has pervaded the evolutionary study of behaviour, and in particular human behaviour, over the last decade. I am grateful to both for providing me with an opportunity to indulge a passion for popular science writing over the years, as well as for allowing me to reuse these pieces in this volume. I also thank the
Observer
,
Scotland on Sunday
, the
Times Higher Education Supplement
, the Royal College of Physicians (London), Charles Pasternak and OneWorld Books, and Faber and Faber for permission to reuse individual pieces published by them. Most of these pieces have been substantially edited or adapted for this volume.

Pieces published in the
Scotsman
make up the bulk of chapters 2, 4, 5, 8, 9, 10, 12, 13 and 16, and feature in chapters 3, 6, 11, 14, 17, 19 and 21. Pieces published in
New Scientist
magazine appear in chapters 7, 13, 14 and 21, and make up the bulk of chapters 3, 17, 18, 20 and
[Page 1]
22. A piece published in the
Observer
contributes to chapter 7, and one from
Scotland on Sunday
to chapter 21.

An article from the
Times Higher Education Supplement
makes up the bulk of chapter 15. Part of chapter 3

appeared in
The Science of Morality
(2007; edited by G.

Walker, published by the Royal College of Physicians, London); part of chapter 12 originally appeared in my
The Human Story
(2004, Faber and Faber); and part of chapter 15 appeared in
What Makes Us Human
(2007; edited by Charles Pasternak, published by OneWorld Books, Oxford).

Finally, I am grateful to my agent Sheila Ableman, and to my editor at Faber, Julian Loose.
[Page 2]

Chapter 1
In the Beginning

We share a history, you and I. A history in which our respective stories snake back through time, edging ever closer to each other until finally they meet up in a common ancestor. Perhaps our lineages meet up only a few generations back, or maybe it was a thousand years ago.

Perhaps it was so long ago that it predates history – though even that could not have been more than two hundred thousand years ago, a mere twinkle in earth time. For we modern humans all descend from a common ancestor who roamed the plains of Africa a mere ten thousand generations ago, ten thousand mothers giving birth to ten thousand daughters... no more than would fit in a town of very modest size today.

For us, that has two important implications. One is that we share most of our traits in common. From Alaska to Tasmania, and Tierra del Fuego to Spitzbergen, we are a single family, one biological species united by common ancestry. The other is that those traits we share are, nonetheless, the product of evolution, honed by the demands of the lives that our ancestors led. Sometimes, they are the product of deep evolutionary time, traits we share with the other members of our biological family,
[Page 3]
the great apes, and especially the African great apes.

Sometimes, those traits are of more recent origin, wrought in the fire of the particular circumstances that our more immediate ancestors faced in the battle for life, traits that mark us out as human – not special, because we are just one of many tens of thousands of individually unique species of animals, but unique in that we alone possess them. Some of these give us the capacity for culture, that remarkable product of the human mind that has made us what we are – those traits that allowed us to break away from our biological roots, that allowed human history to be what it is.

Yet, in our enthusiasm for the wonders of human culture, we sometimes overlook just how much of our behaviour is rooted in our biological evolution. The human mind is surely one of the wonders of the natural world, yet sometimes it seems so pedestrian and constrained that it is hard to see how we differ from any of the other primates. We live in massive conurbations numbering tens of millions of individuals, a product of our cultural flexibility if ever there was one. We have lived in villages only for the last ten thousand years, and cities the size of Bombay or Rio de Janeiro only for the last century at most. These are novel innovations, a product of our capacity to invent ways of making do.

Yet, at the same time, our social world is still what it was several hundred thousand years ago. The number of people we know personally, whom we can trust, whom we feel some emotional affinity for, is no more than 150, Dunbar’s Number. It has been 150 for as long as we have been a species. And it is 150 because our minds lack the capacity to make it any larger. We are
[Page 4]
as much the product of our evolutionary history as any other species is.

I probably owe my interest in evolution to my American grandmother. Though a fiercely God-fearing Presbyterian missionary, she was also a surgeon and sufficiently well-versed in science to be an enthusiast for the new discoveries in human evolution that were emerging from Africa during the 1950s. When I was ten or eleven, she sent me a series of Audubon Society booklets on every imaginable subject to do with the natural world, complete with sticky stamps to paste in. One was on evolution, and covered everything from dinosaurs to humans. I became hooked on the story of human evolution. Some years later, I read Darwin’s
Origin of Species
, having found it by chance in the school library. It was interesting, but I can’t say I got a great deal out of it at the time. I was becoming more interested in philosophy, and science wasn’t really my thing.

Then, five or six years later as a postgraduate student, I was thrust willy-nilly back into Darwin’s world. I was deeply engaged in studying the behaviour of monkeys in the wild, spending several years doing fieldwork in Africa during the early 1970s. At the time, evolutionary thinking in the behavioural sciences was apt to be somewhat loose and wayward. We returned from fieldwork in Ethiopia in late 1975 to find the world had been turned upside down. Edward O. Wilson had just published his
Sociobiology: The New Synthesis
and Richard Dawkins would publish
The Selfish Gene
the following year. It was a life-changing experience for all of us. Overnight, we were made to think about evolutionary processes in a much more rigorous way. We were being asked to return
[Page 5]
to a more strictly Darwinian view, after decades of increasingly lax, often speculative, thinking that had come to characterise much of organismic biology in mid-century.

Of course, neither book invented something that was novel. What both, in their different ways, did was to lay out in stark detail the ideas that evolutionary biologists had slowly been developing over the previous decades.

The big intellectual change was a shift away from thinking that evolution was for the benefit of the species to one in which evolution was for the benefit of the genes that underpinned a trait, whether that trait was physical or behavioural. This should not be taken to imply that behaviour is hardwired, determined by the genes you inherit.

Other books

Lost in Plain Sight by Marta Perry
Unwrapping Mr. Roth by Holley Trent
Spirit Ascendancy by E. E. Holmes
Blades of Winter by G. T. Almasi
A Sudden Sun by Trudy Morgan-Cole
Remember Me by Derek Hansen