The Genius in All of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, and IQ (18 page)

Read The Genius in All of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, and IQ Online

Authors: David Shenk

Tags: #Psychology, #Cognitive Psychology & Cognition, #Cognitive Psychology

BOOK: The Genius in All of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, and IQ
10.19Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

This was an understandable view in 1998, but now we know better. We know now that there are no real “direct genetic effects” and that the nature/nurture distinction is a false one.

Saddled by the old view of genetics, Harris believed that 50 percent of a person’s character comes straight from his or her genes, while most of the rest comes from what behavioral psychologists were calling
“non-shared” environment—a term proposed by geneticist Robert Plomin to explain not-yet-understood environmental influences
. The ambiguous word “non-shared” was designed to convey the opposite of shared family experiences that researchers had assumed affected siblings in similar ways. Non-shared experiences, they reasoned, would affect siblings differently. Much of Harris’s book is an effort to convince the world that peers are the crucial non-shared influence in kids’ lives.

Two years after her book came out, though, it turned out that there was a problem with the shared/non-shared paradigm
. An analysis in 2000 by the University of Virginia psychologist and expert in behavioral genetics Eric Turkheimer revealed that it was another false distinction. Just like “nature/nurture” was supposed to separate genetic effects from environmental effects, “shared” and “non-shared” implied that it was either/or: either people would have similar reactions to shared experiences
or
they would have different reactions to non-shared experiences. Turkheimer’s powerful meta-analysis revealed the much more common third possibility: most of the time, kids have different reactions to shared experiences. (As Turkheimer put it more clinically: “Non-shared environmental variability predominates not because of the systematic effects of environmental events that are not shared among siblings, but rather because of the unsystematic effects of
all
environmental events.”)

Harvard psychologist Howard Gardner had an even more fundamental problem with Harris’s notion of uninfluential parents
. “When we consider the empirical part of Harris’s argument,” he wrote in the
New York Review of Books
, “we find it is indeed true that the research on parent-child socialization is not what we would hope for. However, this says less about parents and children and more about the state of psychological research, particularly with reference to ‘softer variables’ such as affection and ambition. While psychologists have made genuine progress in the study of visual perception and measurable progress in the study of cognition, we do not really know what to look for or how to measure human personality traits, individual emotions, and motivations, let alone character.”

“My reading of the research,” Gardner continued, “suggests that, on the average, parents and peers will turn out to have complementary roles: parents are more important when it comes to education, discipline, responsibility, orderliness, charitableness, and ways of interacting with authority figures. Peers are more important for learning cooperation, for finding the road to popularity, for inventing styles of interaction among people of the same age. Youngsters may find their peers more interesting, but they will look to their parents when contemplating their own futures …
I would give much weight to the hundreds of studies pointing toward parental influence and to the folk wisdom accumulated by hundreds of societies over thousands of years
. And I would, accordingly, be skeptical of a perspective, such as Ms. Harris’s, that relies too heavily on heritability statistics and manages to reanalyze numerous studies and practices so that they all somehow point to the peer group.”

So yes, parents matter
. Parenting isn’t everything or the only thing. Parents don’t have anything close to complete control and in most cases should not shoulder all the blame when things don’t turn out well. But parenting does matter. And to the extent that parents can have a serious impact on the goals, strategies, and personal philosophies of their children, here are four key guideposts to excellence:

1. BELIEVE

In 1931, a young Japanese violinist and instructor named Shinichi Suzuki was teaching a violin class composed mostly of young men. After class one day he was approached by the father of a four-year-old boy: would he consider teaching the gentleman’s son?

Suzuki was startled and dumbfounded. He had no idea if a four-year-old could learn to play violin and little idea how to instruct him. While rehearsing shortly afterward, though, a profound thought struck him: virtually all Japanese children learn to speak Japanese—early, and with precision. “The children of Osaka speak the difficult Osaka dialect,” Suzuki thought to himself. “[They] are unable to speak the Tohoku dialect, but the Tohoku children speak it.
Isn’t that something of an accomplishment?

The obvious lesson, Suzuki surmised, was this: through extraordinary repetition, parental persistence, and strong cultural reinforcement, every young child masters this steep technical challenge. Why couldn’t this lesson apply just as directly to music?

So Suzuki did accept four-year-old Toshiya Eto as a pupil and began to develop a method of instruction he called the “mother-tongue method.” He emphasized heavy parent involvement, steady practice, memorization, and lots of patience. (In retrospect, the parallels between Suzuki’s approach and young Mozart’s musical development are uncanny.) Little Toshiya Eto responded beautifully, prompting Suzuki to recruit more young pupils and refine his methods further.
He came to quickly believe, in fact, that early musical training has an overwhelming advantage over later training and that it was a gateway to an enlightened life
.

He also began to attract attention. A few years into his radical experiment, Suzuki featured seven-year-old Toshiya and several other young students in a public performance. A local newspaper became fixated on the marvels of three-year-old Koji Toyoda, who played one of Dvořák’s “Humoresques” on a one-sixteenth-size violin. “A Genius Appears!” ran the headline. Suzuki was horrified by this interpretation. “[Before the concert], I had told journalists:
talent is not inherent or inborn, but trained and educated
… I had put emphasis on this and had repeated it.” The message was just as important to Suzuki as his method: gifts and talents, he was convinced, were not exclusive to the privileged few; with the right training and persistence, anyone could achieve remarkable success.

As his first young pupil, Toshiya Eto, developed into a world-renowned musician, Suzuki continued to refine his methods and spread their application. By 1949,
his Talent Education Research Institute had thirty-five branches in Japan and was teaching fifteen hundred children
.
The Suzuki method became a sensation around the world and helped transform our understanding of young children’s capabilities
.

It begins with a simple faith that each child has enormous potential and that it is up to us to muster whatever resources we can to exploit that potential. Rather than wonder if their child is among the “gifted” chosen few, parents should believe deeply in the extraordinary potential of their children. Without that parental faith, it is highly unlikely that significant achievement will occur.

2. SUPPORT, DON’T SMOTHER

Imagine, for a moment, that the day your child is born, the doctor gives you a choice of two infant nutritional supplements. The first will transform your child into an astonishing prodigy who, in adulthood, will probably fall back into mediocrity and possibly develop severe emotional problems. The second will produce an emotionally balanced child who is highly unlikely to be a tiny star athlete or musician early on, but who will slowly gather the tools to become a confident, enlightened person with solid relationships and a deep belief in the value of hard work. In the long run, he will have the resources to achieve greatness as an adult.

This stark choice may seem a little absurd, but unconsciously, it is the choice that many parents make.

“You could call it the Britney Spears Syndrome,” says Columbia University psychiatrist Peter Freed. “I see it frequently in my practice—a clear model for how the narcissistic parent injures a child’s sense of self by attaching high-achievement to love.”

It all begins, explains Freed, with a parent who has grown up believing that, in order to be liked, he must be exceptional in some way. The parent subsequently showers his own children with affection after each accomplishment and shuns them after failure. “The parent beams when the child performs well, and then withdraws love when he’s underperforming,” says Freed. “The kid becomes addicted to pleasing the parent. When he doesn’t live up to the parent’s expectations, he feels his parent go cold, which of course is totally devastating. That on-again off-again feeling about how love works sets the stage for narcissism.”
In early adulthood, Freed explains, the child will inevitably struggle with social and emotional challenges (as everyone does) and find that he doesn’t have a deep emotional reservoir to fall back on
. The foundations of love and trust are corrupted by what he experienced as a child. The child victim of a narcissistic parent frequently has a difficult time forming stable life partnerships.

The flip side, says Freed, is a parent who offers unconditional and unshifting love that is decidedly not connected to achievement. “Non-narcissistic parents follow the child’s lead,” he explains. “They’re very good at limit-setting and setting high expectations, but they will wait to see what the kid wants to do and not become anxious if he isn’t high-achieving early on. Their attitude is that the most important thing you’re doing in childhood is making friends and being an active part of the community. If the team wins, they’ll be happy, but if the team has trouble, they’ll have them over and watch a movie.”

There is, in other words, a right way and a wrong way to direct your kids toward achievement. Early exposure to resources is wonderful, as is setting high expectations and demonstrating persistence and resilience when it comes to life challenges. But
a parent must not use affection as a reward for success or a punishment for failure
. The parent must show faith in the child’s ability to seek achievement for his or her own inner satisfaction.

3. PACE AND PERSIST

“It’s not that I’m so smart,” Albert Einstein once said. “It’s just that I stay with problems longer.”

Einstein’s simple statement is a clarion call for all who seek greatness, for themselves or their children. In the end, persistence is
the
difference between mediocrity and enormous success.

The big question is, can it be taught? Can persistence be nurtured by parents and mentors?

Boston College’s Ellen Winner insists not.
Persistence, she argues, “must have an inborn, biological component
.” But the evidence indicates otherwise.
The brain circuits that modulate a person’s level of persistence are plastic—they
can
be altered
. “The key is intermittent reinforcement,” says Robert Cloninger, a Washington University biologist. “A person who grows up getting too frequent rewards will not have persistence, because they’ll quit when the rewards disappear.”

This jibes well with Anders Ericsson’s finding about deliberate practice and with the ascetic philosophy of Kenyan runners: an emphasis on instant gratification makes for bad habits and no effective long-term plan. The ability to delay gratification opens up a whole new vista for anyone looking to better herself.

It also conjures up
a classic study by Stanford psychologist Walter Mischel
, who in the early 1970s offered a group of four-year-olds a choice: they could have one marshmallow immediately or wait a short while (until the researcher got back from an “errand”) for two marshmallows. The results:

 
  • One-third of the kids immediately took the single marshmallow.
  • One-third waited a few minutes but then gave in and settled for the single marshmallow.
  • One-third patiently waited fifteen minutes for two marshmallows.

At the time, it impressed Mischel and his colleagues that so many very young children had the self-discipline to wait indefinitely for a larger reward. But the real lesson came after fourteen years of Mischel’s own waiting—until his original subjects had taken the SATs and were finishing high school. Comparing the SAT scores of the original nonwaiting (instant gratification) group to the waiting (delayed gratification) group, he found the latter scored an average of 210 points higher. Those with an early capacity for self-discipline and delayed gratification had gone on to much higher academic success. The delayed-gratification kids were also rated as much better able to cope with social and personal problems.

The marshmallow study also demonstrated the ability to develop such skills. In side experiments, researchers transformed kids’ wait times by suggesting how to think of the rewards. When kids staring at real marshmallows were encouraged to imagine them as pictures of marshmallows—making them more abstract in their minds—it lengthened their ability to wait from six to eighteen minutes. (The reverse was also true—kids imagining pictures as real marshmallows had their waiting ability shortened.)

Other books

The Keeper's Curse by Diana Harrison
Family Thang by Henderson, James
Reluctant Demon by Linda Rios-Brook
Mistress of the Night by Bassingthwaite, Don, Gross, Dave
The Road To Jerusalem by Guillou, Jan
Me & My Invisible Guy by Sarah Jeffrey
The Stitching Hour by Amanda Lee
Rivethead by Ben Hamper