Read In a Different Key: The Story of Autism Online

Authors: John Donvan,Caren Zucker

Tags: #History, #Psychology, #Autism Spectrum Disorders, #Psychopathology

In a Different Key: The Story of Autism (54 page)

BOOK: In a Different Key: The Story of Autism
2.32Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Because he saw how people treated her differently when they still thought the words were hers.

And because he had so loved hearing everything she had to say.

The dream was that powerful.

35

AN ELUSIVE DEFINITION

“I
t forced me to redefine autism.” When Douglas Biklen spoke these words, in 1990, about facilitated communication, he did not realize how insolent it sounded to the psychiatrists and psychologists who had labored in the field for decades. With nothing more than a degree in special education, Biklen was challenging
“the received knowledge coming from almost fifty years of energetic and sustained research into the condition of autism.” To the established experts, it was appalling that Biklen, whom Eric Schopler called
an “ideologue,” would dare claim the right to
redefine
autism.

And yet that was exactly what the experts had been doing since the time of Leo Kanner—debating one another and jockeying to advance their own definitions and redefinitions of autism: its shape, its scope, its causes. This continuing push and pull happened mostly out of sight of the lay public, which assumed that when people with PhDs and MDs used the word “autism,” they all meant the same thing. But that was never true, because the definition of autism has always been malleable—determined by consensus, or by whoever among the jockeying experts was most persuasive at any given moment. All the big ideas about autism over the years—from mother blaming to concepts with more solid science behind them, like mindblindness, weak central coherence, and the unifying spectrum—were still only hypotheses. Yet most were treated, at the height of their popularity, as objective fact. At least until the next big idea came along.

Given the elasticity of the definition, it has been easy for competing interests to cast autism as what they want it to be, which has at times led to enormous strife. Over the decades, autism has been seen as a
behavioral, or a medical, or a psychogenic challenge. It has been blamed on mothers and it has been blamed on vaccines. It has been answered with “substitute mothering,” megavitamins, facilitated communication, slaps in the face, hugging therapy, and, in some African countries, with exorcism. At this writing, autism is viewed as occurring on a spectrum, a concept that has the virtue of promoting inclusiveness but has also caused acrimony between those who see autism as a tragedy and those who claim it as a gift and an identity.

For years, many of the most respected figures in the autism community have complained about the lack of a single, incontrovertible definition of autism. In 1968, for example, Michael Rutter lamented the
“hopelessly confused state of affairs…more confusing than it need be.” A full ten years later, in 1978, Eric Schopler warned that scientists were critically misunderstanding one another’s work
“because criteria for diagnosis are different.” One more decade after that, in 1988, Fred Volkmar and Donald Cohen wrote in frustration about the
“long and controversial history of the concepts surrounding autism.” And in 1998, Volkmar and Ami Klin complained about
“the confusion and the plethora of diagnostic concepts” surrounding autism, and, by then, Asperger’s syndrome as well.

One of the autism community’s favorite adages has long been “If you’ve met one person with autism, you’ve met
one
person with autism,” which captures the essence of the problem even as it affirms the complex variability of autistic traits. It is this enormous variability of autistic traits that makes defining autism so challenging. That was what hampered Victor Lotter’s effort to count people with autism in the early 1960s, when he had to draw a line to define autism that even he considered arbitrary, or at least subjective, given that it was based on only his own observations. Nevertheless, the subjective judgments of Lotter and other researchers had real consequences, especially in the field of epidemiology. Valid comparisons among populations in different eras and places are impossible when definitions are highly inconsistent. Lotter thought the lesson worth sharing in 1966, when he cautioned that the true prevalence of autism would remain impossible to determine until everyone agreed on what it was they were trying to count.

Debates about who counted as autistic were not only academic; they had a profound effect on the lives of individuals. After all, a person must be diagnosed with autism before he or she can gain access to support services.

Repeatedly, one researcher or another would design a set of criteria intended to bring an end to the chaos—proposing a definition or working model meant to stand as the last word on the matter of what is meant by
autism
. The effort put into this was enormous. The goal, however, remained elusive.


M
ILDRED
C
REAK HAD
tried to
bring order to chaos in 1961, when she published her Nine Points. These were quickly found wanting by the clinical world, which tore them to shreds. She likened the experience to feeling as if she were “throwing them
to the wolves.”

Michael Rutter also tried to make sense of the mess in 1972, when he successfully campaigned to retire the diagnosis of “childhood schizophrenia,” which had long competed with the diagnosis of autism in children. It was “high time,” Rutter argued, for that troublesome concept to be
“politely and respectfully, but firmly, put into its proper place in the section on ‘the history of psychiatry.’ ” Still unsatisfied, he produced a new framework for autism, which became known as the
“Rutter criteria,” in the early 1970s. Based on Rutter’s and other researchers’ epidemiological and clinical studies,
the criteria specified that autism manifested itself in three domains, in varying degree—through language impairment; social impairment; and restrictive, repetitive patterns of behavior. An inspiration for Lorna Wing and Judith Gould’s later “triad of impairments,” the Rutter criteria gained a following, and for several years thereafter they were often cited by researchers as the standard they relied on. Some of this acceptance was due, no doubt, to Rutter’s standing as one of academic psychiatry’s most lucid thinkers and writers. He was adept at defining the parameters of a topic, and he had a clear, unpretentious style that made his conclusions that much more forceful.
“The question was not how to differentiate autism from normality. The hospital porter could do that
without skilled assistance,” he once wrote, on the challenge of clarifying what autism looked like in children seen at the Maudsley.

By 1977, however, the
National Society for Autistic Children was promoting its own definition, which added a fourth domain: sensory processing. Developed by UCLA researchers Edward Ritvo and B. J. Freeman, this definition made
“abnormal responses to sensations” also central to the experience of having autism.

And then there was
the American Psychiatric Association’s
DSM
, which constituted its own saga.


I
N
1993,
WHEN
Fred Volkmar and his colleagues were trying to get Asperger’s syndrome into the
DSM
, the book had already been in print for
more than forty years, with revised editions published whenever psychiatry changed its mind, which was surprisingly often.

After 1980, for example, the
DSM
was overhauled at least once a decade, with whole diagnoses heavily revised or dropped altogether, and new diagnoses added. Most famously, after much pressure from the gay community and its allies, and despite the vociferous objections of a contingent of mostly older psychiatrists, the
DSM
retired the diagnosis of homosexuality as a mental illness in 1972. Though this and other amendments were billed as the positive consequences of better research and understanding, they revealed the extent to which psychiatric diagnosis is influenced not only by scientific data but by cultural, political, and other factors.

Autism was not included in the
DSM
until 1980, in the edition known as
DSM-III
, which appeared nearly forty years after Leo Kanner published his first paper on the condition. It proved to be a protean definition—altered significantly in 1987, and again in 1994. Although it was changed only slightly in 2000, it underwent a dramatic rewrite, accompanied by much angst, in 2013. Throughout this process, the length of the definition waxed and waned. The number of words in its symptom checklist, originally about seventy, jumped to more than six hundred in one edition, shrank to three hundred in the next, and then expanded again, to almost nine hundred words, two editions later. The
name kept changing too—from
Infantile Autism
to
Autistic Disorder
to
Autism Spectrum Disorder
. Most critically, the symptom checklist the
DSM
writers developed to identify those with autism kept being revised. In one edition, a patient had to match a minimum of 8 out of 16 criteria to receive the diagnosis. In another, he or she had to match at least 6 out of 12. In the second case, according to Volkmar and Brian Reichow at Yale, there were more than two thousand different symptom combinations that could produce an autism diagnosis. The addition of Asperger’s disorder as a separate diagnosis in 1994 further complicated the diagnostic picture.

Very often, the
DSM
’s zigzags on autism have reflected the almost philosophical divide between two camps of experts: the “lumpers” and the “splitters.” This tension was on display when Lorna Wing and Hans Asperger disagreed as to whether his cases and Leo Kanner’s were more alike than different, or the other way around. Asperger, it will be remembered, believed that he saw two distinctly different syndromes in his cases and Kanner’s cases. His insistence on splitting his and Kanner’s children into separate groups, according to key differences, made him a classic splitter. Wing, on the other hand, always spoke like a true lumper, arguing that virtually everyone with some trace or combination of autistic behaviors belonged to the same group, since the core mechanisms of autistic behavior operated in all of them in the same way, albeit to varying degrees. This thinking is what led to her construction of the highly influential “spectrum” model of autism. Today this quintessentially lumper idea is widely accepted, to the point where even popular culture appears to embrace it as unassailable truth.

But if history is a guide, that could change. As with any previous understanding of autism, the spectrum construct is not without drawbacks, and a swing back to the splitters is quite possible if, for example, further scientific inquiry determines that clusters of autistic behaviors that
look
similar in fact arise from divergent causes—environmental, genetic, or other. The study of cancer offers an analogy. For years “cancer” survived as an umbrella term for all cancers, until the field of oncology learned that different cancers are in fact radically different, each with its own signature and its own distinct biological process—and was thus forced down the path of the splitters. Autism too might
turn out to be a plural entity. Researchers might discover that there are in fact
autisms
, each with its own genetic “fingerprint,” each significantly unrelated to the others. If that were to happen, the idea of a broad spectrum, which smudges those differences, would have less explanatory power, and might lose some of its currency in the conversation about autism.

Such lines of inquiry, however, would require a commitment to research into the biomedical aspects of autism rather than a focus on psychiatric or behavioral interventions. Yet at the beginning of the 1990s, there was virtually no biological research into autism, and the few scientists studying it had little financial support. That is, until four parents, living on opposite coasts of the United States, decided to do something about it.

36

MEETING OF THE MINDS

T
he two husbands were arguing before the appetizers arrived. By the meal’s end, it appeared the merger idea was in trouble. For reasons of philosophy and personality, it was beginning to look obvious that two of the most ambitious parent organizations ever to join the autism cause would not, after all, be getting married.

It was disappointing, perhaps, because both couples and both organizations were after exactly the same goal: to inspire the biomedical research community to take autism seriously. Because as of their sit-down in late 1995, at a restaurant near Princeton University, that was definitely not happening.

Not biology-based researchers anyway. Not the geneticists, the allergists, the endocrinologists, or the gastroenterologists. For researchers in these and other biomedical fields, autism was a non-topic, a dead-end children’s condition best left to the psychologists, psychiatrists, and pediatricians.

By remarkable coincidence, these two sets of parents, on opposite coasts of the United States, had both recently set out to alter this reality with a level of ambition and energy that would, together, galvanize a movement. Unaware the other existed, both couples had had the same idea for changing the world, at almost exactly the same time.

One couple lived in Hillsborough, New Jersey, but used a mailbox in nearby Princeton for their group’s address. Husband and wife were both Ivy Leaguers, and they liked the implied association with Princeton University. The other couple, who had flown in from Los Angeles for this lunch, both made their living in the movie industry. A
mutual acquaintance had encouraged the four of them to get together at this table to discuss joining forces. On the face of it, this meeting of the minds made perfect sense.

After all, they had
so much in common.


K
AREN
L
ONDON, A
corporate lawyer, had stopped practicing law when her son, Zach, was diagnosed with autism in 1987. Her husband, Eric, a research psychiatrist, had continued his work, which
focused on Alzheimer’s disease and geriatric psychiatry, although now, of course, he read anything new hitting the medical literature that shed light on his son’s condition. Colleagues like his friend and fellow psychiatrist Arvan Mirrow also kept an eye out for relevant research and brought it to Eric’s attention now and then.

BOOK: In a Different Key: The Story of Autism
2.32Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Other books

Santa Sleuth by Kathi Daley
The Infinite Plan by Isabel Allende
Randalls Round by Eleanor Scott
The Destroyer Goddess by Laura Resnick
Los hornos de Hitler by Olga Lengyel
Lucena by Mois Benarroch
Wolf Among Wolves by Hans Fallada
Whispers in the Dawn by Aurora Rose Lynn