How Dogs Love Us: A Neuroscientist and His Adopted Dog Decode the Canine Brain (3 page)

BOOK: How Dogs Love Us: A Neuroscientist and His Adopted Dog Decode the Canine Brain
12.3Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

On top of her speed, Callie exhibited a laserlike focus on squirrels. Once caught in her field of view, a squirrel would be chased up a tree in sheer terror with Callie scrabbling at the bark. The treeing of squirrels wasn’t the main characteristic of terriers, and although in all likelihood Callie was a mongrel, I couldn’t help but hold on to the fantasy that we had found an unrecognized purebred at the shelter.

The treeing of squirrels turned out to be the key to decoding Callie’s heritage. Not being native to Georgia, I was unaware of a breed peculiar to the South: the treeing feist. According to the American Treeing Feist Association, the treeing feist, or mountain feist, existed in the southern Appalachians long before rat terriers were brought to America. While terriers were bred to catch vermin, feists were bred to hunt. And while squirrels are their primary prey, the feist will gladly hunt raccoons, rabbits, or birds. With longer legs than terriers, feists are built for silent speed. They live to tree a squirrel until its owner comes to catch it. The feist has a storied history intertwined with the beginnings of the country. George Washington wrote about
them in his diary, and Abraham Lincoln even referred to them in a poem.

Over the next two days, Callie’s personality began to emerge. And even though we had changed her name, somehow the shelter had been closer to the mark. She loved to eat.

Every day when I came home from work, Callie would burst into the room and jump up and down like a pogo stick, wagging her tail furiously, her eyes filled with pure joy. But on the fourth day, when I came home, she just lay on the rug, hardly moving at all.

“What’s wrong with Callie?” I yelled out to Kat, who was busy helping Helen with homework.

“What do you mean?” Kat asked.

“She’s just lying here on the floor.” This brought everyone running into the room.

Maddy covered her mouth as her eyes began to tear up.

“What’s wrong with her?” Helen asked.

Callie just rolled on her side and began to whimper.

I kneeled down to see what was wrong. Her belly was bloated out of all proportion to her rail-thin figure. When I touched her tummy, she squirmed away and made a little whine.

We immediately set to looking for what she might have eaten. I expected to find the tattered remnants of a shoe or one of the kids’ toys. Ten minutes of searching yielded nothing, and Callie just seemed to be getting worse. Moving from sitting to standing to lying down, she was unable to find a position that didn’t cause her pain.

Finally Kat yelled out from the kitchen pantry. “I found it!”

This is where we kept the dog food. Over the years, we had learned that it was cheapest to buy fifty-pound bags. For ease of access, we stored the food in a large plastic bin on the floor of the pantry. With the lid on, the bin had successfully kept the dogs from helping themselves. Until now.

The lid was pried off, and a few bits of kibble were scattered about. Callie had somehow figured out how to open the container and had gorged herself into oblivion. There was still plenty of food left in the bin, but then again, none of us knew how much had been there before she started. The bits on the floor indicated that she had eaten so much that she hadn’t bothered to pick up the remaining scraps.

Helen was on the verge of panic. “We have to take her to the vet!”

I looked at the clock. Past six. Kat was thinking the same thing: after-hours emergency visit. This was going to be expensive.

Callie was such a thin, little dog, and her belly was like a balloon. It was hard to imagine how all that stuff was going to make it through. Maybe she ate so much that she tore her stomach. Was that possible? I had heard of such things happening in humans, but never in dogs.

“Do you know what happened to the rawhides?” Kat asked.

“What rawhides?”

“The pack of rawhides I bought yesterday.”

As Callie writhed on the floor, we both knew the answer.

We headed to the emergency veterinary clinic. This was a fully staffed, multispecialty hospital, manned 24/7 and arrayed with the latest medical technologies. But unlike a human hospital, this was strictly pay in advance. Two hundred dollars to open a tab.

We weren’t exactly sure what else Callie had eaten, so the first order of business was an X-ray.

“You see that?” asked the vet as she pointed to what looked like the silhouette of a dog filled with popcorn. “That’s all food. The good news is that there aren’t any foreign objects.”

“And the bad news?”

“She can’t really drink anything in this state. If she gets dehydrated, the food could turn to concrete in her stomach, which will make it very hard to pass. I recommend we give her an IV to keep her hydrated and keep her overnight.”

None of us wanted to leave our new pet overnight in the hospital. Helen summed it up: “Daddy, she just came from the shelter. She’s really scared.”

“Can’t you just make her vomit?” I asked the vet. That was clearly not where she had intended the conversation to go.

“We can try,” she replied with some resignation. “But it doesn’t usually work at this stage.”

Since there wasn’t much of a risk from trying, Callie received an injection of apomorphine, a potent emetic. Within five minutes she began retching. But, as the vet had predicted, nothing came up. Callie just looked confused and frightened.

There wasn’t any choice. With tears all around, we said good-bye to Little Miss Piggy and trundled out of the hospital. Even though she had been with us only a few days, I couldn’t help but feel that we had somehow failed her. What kind of pet owners were we if our new adoptee landed in the hospital within the first week?

The next morning, the hospital called to say that Callie’s vital signs were stable but that she hadn’t passed anything yet. They recommended keeping her another twenty-four hours.

“Can we take her home?” I asked.

“We don’t recommend it.”

Helen pulled at my sleeve, begging me to pick her up.

Kat and I figured that if anything bad was going to happen, it would have happened already. Besides, Callie had the benefit of being rehydrated by IV, which we hoped would keep her tanked up until the food made its way through.

At the hospital, we had to sign Callie out “against medical advice.” Yes, we were very bad dog owners. When we got home, Callie bounded into the house as if nothing had happened. She drank a bunch of water and ran outside to porpoise through the ivy.

We had medical insurance from the animal shelter through the first thirty days of the adoption, but the insurance company denied
the claim. Some fine print about covering only foreign-body ingestions, not pathologic overeating.

It didn’t matter. I was just grateful Callie was okay. And she would soon change my life, helping answer my questions about what Newton had felt and eventually revealing clues to the deeper question: What are dogs really thinking?

2

What It’s Like to Be a Dog

T
HE IDEA OF SCANNING DOGS

BRAINS
didn’t occur to me all at once. As with most scientific developments, it started as a series of random thoughts and inferences that eventually led to an aha moment. While Newton’s death planted the seed of an idea, it was my own discomfort around groups of people that helped it grow.

For the past fifteen years, my lab has used brain-scanning technology to understand how the human reward system works. The main tool that we use is magnetic resonance imaging, or MRI. About the size of a car, an MRI scanner is pretty much a large tube wrapped in miles of wire. When electricity is sent through the wire, it creates a powerful magnetic field that can be used to see inside of a person’s brain. A standard MRI, like what you would get if you went to a hospital, takes a picture of your brain. Scientists soon discovered that if you took several pictures of the brain in rapid fire, you could see the brain in action. This is called
functional MRI
, or fMRI, and it opened the black box of the human mind. With fMRI, we can measure activity inside the brain while a person is actually doing something, like
reading or doing math or even while experiencing different types of emotions. This allows scientists to figure out how the brain actually works (hence the
functional
in fMRI).

As the leader of a research lab, it is one of my duties to hold an annual lab party. You would think that this would be an enjoyable activity. Inevitably it is a source of stress in our household. The dogs don’t help either.

Like me, the dogs were never properly socialized to large groups of people, something for which I take full blame. Since we don’t have parties often, it seemed unreasonable to make the dogs learn how to behave in such situations. Nevertheless, one cannot completely abdicate these social necessities, as with our once-a-year gathering of lab members.

Ignoring my antipathy, Kat and the girls threw themselves into the preparations for the annual party. They brought all the chairs out of the dining room and created a semicircular seating arrangement in the family room. Nothing unusual about this, presuming that the guests are able adults who can manage conversation while eating and drinking without tables to place their food upon. It does not, however, account for dogs either underfoot, in the case of Callie, or swishing big, fluffy tails around, in the case of Lyra.

If everyone was a dog person, these parties wouldn’t present a problem. In recent years, I have certainly become more selective in allowing people to work in the lab, and this includes my asking whether he or she is a dog person or, second best, a cat person. But can you really trust someone who doesn’t have a pet? Despite my best efforts to fill the lab with animal lovers, I have no control over spouses and partners.

Kat wanted to lock Lyra and Callie in the bedroom when the guests arrived. The dogs weren’t accustomed to being locked up, so I feigned ignorance and let them have free run of the party. As guests
arrived, Callie would give a perfunctory woof. Lyra just grinned and wagged her tail excessively as the people filed in.

I could trust the dog people in the lab to keep an eye on the dogs and prevent them from swiping food, so I slipped out to help Kat in the kitchen. She was dishing up the hors d’oeuvres and pouring drinks. The team, while diverse in terms of background, was predominantly American, with the exception of one lab member from India. It was at the moment I stepped into the kitchen when he arrived with his wife.

Their entrance was marked in dramatic fashion by an ear-piercing “Eeeeeee! Eeeeeee! Eeeeeee!”

I rushed out of the kitchen. My colleague’s wife, wrapped in a lovely sari, had backed herself into a corner, shrieking like a bird at the mere sight of the dogs.

This behavior baffled Callie, so she paid no further notice to her and moved on to look for food droppings. Lyra, on the other hand, found these vocalizations highly stimulating. She tracked right to the sound and starting jumping up and down and barking in what appeared to me to be a request to play. But the grimace of terror on the woman’s face indicated no such desire.

I grabbed Lyra by the collar and led her to the bedroom.

“Sorry, girl. You can’t play tonight.”

What did Lyra think was the reason that woman was screaming? If Lyra were a person, I could have simply asked her. How else could I find out what was going through her mind?

To truly know what a dog is thinking, you would have to be a dog.

The question of what a dog is thinking is actually an old metaphysical debate, which has its origins in Descartes’s famous saying
cogito ergo sum
—“I think, therefore I am.” Our entire human experience exists solely inside our heads. Photons may strike our retinas, but it is only through the activity of our brains that we have the subjective experience of seeing a rainbow or the sublime beauty of a sunset over the ocean. Does a dog see those things? Of course. Do they experience them the same way? Absolutely not.

When Lyra was jumping and barking at the woman wrapped in purple, with a red dot on her forehead, Lyra experienced the same things at a primitive level that I did. Purple. Red. Screaming. Those are the sensory primitives. They originate in photons bouncing off dyes, pressure waves in the air around the woman’s vocal cords. But my brain interprets those events one way and Lyra’s brain another.

Observing Lyra’s behavior doesn’t tell us what she was thinking. From past experience, I knew that Lyra barked and jumped in response to different things. She barks when we’re eating. In that context, a natural assumption would be that she wants food too. But she also barks after dropping a tennis ball at my feet. I had no comparable frame of reference for what had attracted her to the screaming woman that night at the party.

The question of what it is like to be a dog could be approached from two very different perspectives. The hard approach asks the question: What is it like for a dog to be a dog? If we could do that, then all the questions about why a dog behaves the way it does would become clear. The problem with being a dog, though, is that we would have no language to describe what we felt. The best we can do is ask the related, but substantially easier question: What would it be like for
us
to be a dog?

By imagining ourselves in the skin of another animal, we can recast questions of behavior into their human equivalent. The question of why Lyra harassed the party guest becomes: If I were Lyra, why would I bark at that woman? Framed that way, we can form all sorts of speculations for dog behavior.

Many authors have written about the dog mind, and some have even attempted to answer the types of questions I have posed. I will not review this vast literature. I will, however, point out that much of it is based on two potentially flawed assumptions—both stemming from the paradox of getting into a dog’s mind without actually being a dog.

The first flaw comes from the human tendency to anthropomorphize, or project our own thoughts and feelings onto things that aren’t ourselves. We can’t help it. Our brains are hardwired to project our thoughts onto other people. This is called
mentalizing
, and it is critical for human social interactions. People are able to interact with each other only because they are constantly guessing what other people are thinking. The brevity of text messages, for example, and the fact that we are able to communicate with less than 140 characters at a time work because people maintain mental models of each other. The actual linguistic content of most text exchanges is minimal. And because humans have common elements of culture, we tend to react in fairly similar ways. For example, if I watch a movie that makes me sad, I can use my own reaction to intuit that the people sitting around me are feeling the same way. I could even start a conversation with a complete stranger based on our shared experience, using my own thoughts as a starting point. But dogs are not the same as humans, and they certainly don’t have a shared culture like we do. There is no avoiding the fact that when we observe dog behavior, we view it through the filter of the human mind. Unfortunately, much of dog literature says more about the human writer than the dog.

Other books

Lindsay's Surprise Crush by Angela Darling
Worlds of BBW Erotic Romance - Box Set by Primrose, Jennie, Demure, Celia
Bouquet for Iris by Diane T. Ashley
By Grace Possessed by Jennifer Blake
Sugar Rush by Rachel Astor
Viral Nation by Grimes, Shaunta
The Dressmaker of Khair Khana by Gayle Tzemach Lemmon
Tempted by Marion, Elise